[1]
A. P. Shimamura and S. E. Palmer, Aesthetic science: connecting minds, brains, and experience. [Online]. Available: http://lib.myilibrary.com/browse/open.asp?id=342328&entityid=https://idp.brunel.ac.uk/entity
[2]
R. Reber, N. Schwarz, and P. Winkielman, ‘Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience?’, Personality and social psychology review, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 364–382, Nov. 2004, doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3.
[3]
R. B. Zajonc, ‘Attitudinal effects of mere exposure’, Journal of personality and social psychology, vol. 9, no. 2, Pt.2, pp. 1–27, 1968, doi: 10.1037/h0025848. [Online]. Available: http://web.ebscohost.com/ehost/viewarticle?data=dGJyMPPp44rp2%2fdV0%2bnjisfk5Ie46a9JsKmuTLek63nn5Kx95uXxjL6nsEevrq1KrqexOLewsEu4qrU4v8OkjPDX7Ivf2fKB7eTnfLujsEixqbJLs6e2TKTi34bls%2bOGpNrgVe7a5j7y1%2bVVv8Skeeyzr1G0rqtJsKavUaumrkmk3O2K69fyVeTr6oTy2%2faM&hid=125
[4]
L. L. Jacoby, C. Kelley, J. Brown, and J. Jasechko, ‘Becoming famous overnight: Limits on the ability to avoid unconscious influences of the past.’, Journal of personality and social psychology, vol. 56, no. 3, pp. 326–338, 1989 [Online]. Available: http://web.ebscohost.com/ehost/viewarticle?data=dGJyMPPp44rp2%2fdV0%2bnjisfk5Ie46a9JsKmuTLek63nn5Kx95uXxjL6nsEevrq1KrqexOK%2bnuEu0sK9OnrfLPvLo34bx1%2bGM5%2bXsgeKzr1Czq65Ir6mvS6Ti34bls%2bOGpNrgVe7a5j7y1%2bVVv8Skeeyzr1G2r6tJta6yUKumrkmk3O2K69fyVeTr6oTy2%2faM&hid=125
[5]
Semir Zeki, ‘Art and the brain’, Daedalus, vol. 127, no. 2, pp. 71–103, 1998 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/20027491
[6]
Ramachandran, V.S., Hirstein, W., ‘The science of art: a neurological theory of aesthetic experience’, Journal of consciousness studies, vol. 6, no. 6–7, pp. 15–51, 1999 [Online]. Available: http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/imp/jcs/1999/00000006/F0020006/949
[7]
H. Leder, B. Belke, A. Oeberst, and D. Augustin, ‘A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments’, British Journal of Psychology, vol. 95, no. 4, pp. 489–508, Nov. 2004, doi: 10.1348/0007126042369811.
[8]
D. D. Cinzia and G. Vittorio, ‘Neuroaesthetics: a review’, Current opinion in neurobiology, vol. 19, no. 6, pp. 682–687, Dec. 2009, doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.09.001.
[9]
T. Jacobsen, ‘The Fine Arts, Neurology, and Neuroscience - New Discoveries and Changing Landscapes’, in The Fine Arts, Neurology, and Neuroscience - New Discoveries and Changing Landscapes, vol. 204, Elsevier, 2013, pp. 159–168.
[10]
S. Zeki and M. Lamb, ‘The neurology of kinetic art’, Brain, vol. 117, no. 3, pp. 607–636, 1994, doi: 10.1093/brain/117.3.607.
[11]
E. B. Goldstein, Sensation and perception, 6th ed. Australia: Wadsworth-Thomson Learning, 2002.
[12]
D. E. Berlyne, Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. Washington, D.C.: Hemisphere Publishing, 1974.
[13]
Gombrich, E. H., Art and illusion: a study in the psychology of pictorial representation, 6th ed., with New preface. London: Phaidon, 2002.
[14]
Rolf Reber, Piotr Winkielman and Norbert Schwarz, ‘Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments’, Psychological science, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 45–48, 1998.
[15]
R. F. Bornstein, ‘Exposure and affect: overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968-1987.’, Psychological bulletin, vol. 106, no. 2, pp. 265–289, 1989, doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.265. [Online]. Available: http://web.ebscohost.com/ehost/viewarticle?data=dGJyMPPp44rp2%2fdV0%2bnjisfk5Ie46a9JsKmuTLek63nn5Kx95uXxjL6nsEevrq1KrqexOLewsEu4qrU4v8OkjPDX7Ivf2fKB7eTnfLujr020q7VLt622TqTi34bls%2bOGpNrgVe7a5j7y1%2bVVv8Skeeyzr1G3pqtIrqqwSqumrkmk3O2K69fyVeTr6oTy2%2faM&hid=125
[16]
P. Winkielman, J. Halberstadt, T. Fazendeiro, and S. Catty, ‘Prototypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind’, Psychological science, vol. 17, no. 9, pp. 799–806, Sep. 2006, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01785.x.
[17]
M. Nadal, M. À. Capó, J. Rosselló, E. Munar, and C. J. Cela-Conde, ‘Towards a framework for the study of the neural correlates of aesthetic preference’, Spatial vision, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 379–396, May 2008, doi: 10.1163/156856808784532653.
[18]
Rudolf Arnheim, ‘On order, simplicity and entropy’, Leonardo, vol. 7, no. 2, pp. 139–141, 1974 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1572794
[19]
T. Jacobsen and L. Hoefel, ‘Descriptive and evaluative judgement processes: Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of processing symmetry and aesthetics.’, Cognitive, affective, & behavioral neuroscience, vol. 3, no. 4, 2003 [Online]. Available: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758%2FCABN.3.4.289#page-1
[20]
Silvia, Paul J., Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina, Greensboro, NC, US, p_silvia@uncg.edu, ‘Emotional responses to art: From collation and arousal to cognition and emotion.’, Review of general psychology [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pdh&AN=2006-01036-003&site=ehost-live
[21]
R. Von Der Heydt, E. Peterhans and G. Baumgartner, ‘Illusory Contours and Cortical Neuron Responses’, Science, vol. 224, no. 4654, pp. 1260–1262, 1984 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org.ezproxy.brunel.ac.uk/stable/1692068
[22]
Colin Martindale, Kathleen Moore and Jonathan Borkum, ‘Aesthetic Preference: Anomalous Findings for Berlyne’s Psychobiological Theory’, The American Journal of Psychology, vol. 103, no. 1, pp. 53–80, 1990 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org.ezproxy.brunel.ac.uk/stable/1423259
[23]
‘doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.08.005 - ledercarbonripsas2006.pdf’. [Online]. Available: http://www.experimental-psychology.de/ccc/docs/pubs/ledercarbonripsas2006.pdf
[24]
‘AugustinLederHutzlerCarbon2008.pdf’. [Online]. Available: http://www.experimental-psychology.org/ccc/docs/pubs/AugustinLederHutzlerCarbon2008.pdf
[25]
M. D. Augustin, B. Defranceschi, H. K. Fuchs, C.-C. Carbon, and F. Hutzler, ‘The neural time course of art perception: An ERP study on the processing of style versus content in art’, Neuropsychologia, vol. 49, no. 7, pp. 2071–2081, Jun. 2011, doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.038.
[26]
D. E. Berlyne, ‘Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value’, Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 8, no. 5, pp. 279–286, Sep. 1970, doi: 10.3758/BF03212593.
[27]
S. F. Checkosky and D. Whitlock, ‘Effects of pattern goodness on recognition time in a memory search task.’, Journal of experimental psychology, vol. 100, no. 2, pp. 341–348, 1973, doi: 10.1037/h0035692. [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=pdh&AN=1974-20231-001&site=ehost-live&scope=site&custid=s1123049
[28]
M. S. Livingstone, ‘Is it warm? Is it real? Or just low spatial frequency?’, Science, vol. 290, no. 5495 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3078215
[29]
H. Kawabata and S. Zeki, ‘Neural correlates of beauty’, Journal of neurophysiology, vol. 91, no. 4, pp. 1699–1705, Apr. 2004, doi: 10.1152/jn.00696.2003.
[30]
‘Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference for paintings’, Neuroreport, 2004 [Online]. Available: http://www.yorku.ca/vgoel/reprints/Vartanian_Goel_art.pdf
[31]
S. Zeki and J. Stutters, ‘A brain-derived metric for preferred kinetic stimuli’, Open biology, vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 120001–120001, Feb. 2012, doi: 10.1098/rsob.120001.
[32]
M. Bar and M. Neta, ‘Humans prefer curved visual objects’, Psychological science, vol. 17, no. 8, pp. 645–648, Aug. 2006, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01759.x.
[33]
J. Sammartino and S. E. Palmer, ‘Aesthetic issues in spatial composition: Effects of vertical position and perspective on framing single objects.’, Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 865–879, 2012 [Online]. Available: http://web.a.ebscohost.com/ehost/detail?vid=3&sid=d5251d3c-e066-483c-819c-82080369407f%40sessionmgr4003&hid=4109&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#db=pdh&AN=2012-06754-001
[34]
S. Palmer, K. B. Schloss, and J. Sammartino, ‘Visual aesthetics and human preference’, Annual review of psychology, 2013 [Online]. Available: http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~plab/pdf/PalmerSchlossSammartino(2013)AR.pdf
[35]
A. C. Hurlbert and Y. Ling, ‘Biological components of sex differences in color preference’, Current biology, vol. 17, no. 16, pp. R623–R625, Aug. 2007, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.022.
[36]
K. B. Schloss, R. M. Poggesi, and S. E. Palmer, ‘Effects of university affiliation and "school spirit” on color preferences: Berkeley versus Stanford’, Psychonomic bulletin & review, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 498–504, Jun. 2011, doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0073-1.
[37]
I. C. McManus, ‘The aesthetics of simple figures’, British journal of psychology, vol. 71, pp. 505–524, 1980 [Online]. Available: http://search.proquest.com/docview/1293705404?OpenUrlRefId=info:xri/sid:summon&accountid=14494
[38]
I. C. McManus and P. Weatherby, ‘The golden section and the aesthetics of from and composition’, Empirical studies of the arts, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 209–232, 1997 [Online]. Available: http://www.ucl.ac.uk/medical-education/reprints/1997EmpiricalStudiesArts-GoldenSection.PDF
[39]
S. Palmer, J. Gardner, and T. Wickens, ‘Aesthetic issues in spatial composition: effects of position and direction on framing single objects’, Spatial vision, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 421–449, May 2008, doi: 10.1163/156856808784532662.
[40]
M. Tucker and R. Ellis, ‘On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions.’, Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, vol. 24, no. 3, pp. 830–846, 1998 [Online]. Available: http://web.b.ebscohost.com/ehost/detail?sid=ad395592-8f4e-44f9-8ce0-42eabf61c6b4%40sessionmgr115&vid=4&hid=120&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#db=pdh&AN=1998-02354-011
[41]
I. C. McManus, ‘Symmetry and asymmetry in aesthetics and the arts’, European review, no. Suppl. S2, pp. 157–180, 2005, doi: 10.1017/S1062798705000736. [Online]. Available: http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displayAbstract?fromPage=online&aid=331700
[42]
M. Jakesch, H. Leder, and M. Forster, ‘Image ambiguity and fluency’, PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 9, Sep. 2013, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074084.
[43]
U. Kirk, ‘The neural basis of object-context relationships on aesthetic judgment’, PLoS ONE, vol. 3, no. 11, Nov. 2008, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003754.
[44]
I. C. McManus, F. A. Zhou, S. l’Anson, L. Waterfield, K. Stöver, and R. Cook, ‘The psychometrics of photographic cropping: The influence of colour, meaning, and expertise’, Perception, 2011, doi: 10.1068/p6700.
[45]
O. Vartanian et al., ‘Impact of contour on aesthetic judgments and approach-avoidance decisions in architecture’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 110, no. Supplement_2, pp. 10446–10453, Jun. 2013, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301227110.
[46]
S. Zeki and T. Ishizu, ‘The "Visual Shock” of Francis Bacon: an essay in neuroesthetics’, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 7, 2013, doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00850.
[47]
Stefan Koelsch, Brain and music. Chichester, West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons, 2012.
[48]
S. Koelsch, ‘Toward a neural basis of music perception – a review and updated model’, Frontier in psychology, vol. 2, 2011, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00110.
[49]
K. Kornysheva, D. Y. von Cramon, T. Jacobsen, and R. I. Schubotz, ‘Tuning-in to the beat: aesthetic appreciation of musical rhythms correlates with a premotor activity boost’, Human brain mapping, p. NA-NA, 2009, doi: 10.1002/hbm.20844.
[50]
C. J. Stevens, ‘Music perception and cognition: a review of recent cross-cultural research’, Topics in cognitive science, vol. 4, no. 4, pp. 653–667, Oct. 2012, doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01215.x.
[51]
Unjung Nam, ‘Pitch distributions in Korean court music: evidence consistent with tonal hierarchies’, Music perception: an interdisciplinary journal, vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 243–247 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40285789
[52]
C. L. Krumhansl and R. N. Shepard, ‘Quantification of the hierarchy of tonal functions within a diatonic context.’, Journal of experimental psychology: human perception and performance, vol. 5, no. 4, pp. 579–594, 1979, doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.5.4.579.
[53]
C. Wacongne, E. Labyt, V. van Wassenhove, T. Bekinschtein, L. Naccache, and S. Dehaene, ‘Evidence for a hierarchy of predictions and prediction errors in human cortex’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 108, no. 51, pp. 20754–20759, Dec. 2011, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117807108. [Online]. Available: http://www.pnas.org/content/108/51/20754.full
[54]
M. Besson and F. Faïta, ‘An event-related potential (ERP) study of musical expectancy: Comparison of musicians with nonmusicians.’, Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, vol. 21, no. 6, pp. 1278–1296, 1995 [Online]. Available: http://web.a.ebscohost.com/ehost/detail?sid=a7648c3a-40e6-45b2-968f-b5892491674d%40sessionmgr4005&vid=4&hid=4212&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#db=pdh&AN=1996-16299-001
[55]
G. Kuhn and Z. Dienes, ‘Implicit learning of nonlocal musical rules: Implicitly learning more than chunks.’, Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, vol. 31, no. 6, pp. 1417–1432, 2005 [Online]. Available: http://web.a.ebscohost.com/ehost/pdfviewer/pdfviewer?sid=ce7eb9dd-4212-4b81-8707-d91549eba95e%40sessionmgr4003&vid=5&hid=4109
[56]
G. Orgs, K. Lange, J.-H. Dombrowski, and M. Heil, ‘Conceptual priming for environmental sounds and words: an ERP study’, Brain and cognition, vol. 62, no. 3, pp. 267–272, Dec. 2006, doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.05.003.
[57]
S. Nozaradan, I. Peretz, and A. Mouraux, ‘Selective Neuronal Entrainment to the Beat and Meter Embedded in a Musical Rhythm’, Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 32, no. 49, pp. 17572–17581, Dec. 2012, doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3203-12.2012.
[58]
B. Calvo-Merino, C. Urgesi, G. Orgs, S. M. Aglioti, and P. Haggard, ‘Extrastriate body area underlies aesthetic evaluation of body stimuli’, Experimental brain research, vol. 204, no. 3, pp. 447–456, Jul. 2010, doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2283-6.
[59]
G. Rhodes, ‘The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty’, Annual review of psychology, vol. 57, pp. 199–266, 2006 [Online]. Available: http://www.annualreviews.org/doi/abs/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208
[60]
Peelen, Marius V.1Downing, Paul E.1 p.downing@bangor.ac.uk, ‘The neural basis of visual body perception.’, Nature reviews neuroscience, vol. 8, no. 8, pp. 636–648 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=a9h&AN=25811795&site=ehost-live
[61]
D. A. Minnebusch and I. Daum, ‘Neuropsychological mechanisms of visual face and body perception’, Neuroscience & biobehavioral reviews, vol. 33, no. 7, pp. 1133–1144, Jul. 2009, doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.008.
[62]
C. Di Dio, E. Macaluso, and G. Rizzolatti, ‘The golden beauty: brain response to classical and renaissance sculptures’, PLoS ONE, vol. 2, no. 11, Nov. 2007, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001201.
[63]
C. Lambrou, D. Veale, and G. Wilson, ‘The role of aesthetic sensitivity in body dysmorphic disorder.’, Journal of abnormal psychology, vol. 120, no. 2, pp. 443–453, 2011 [Online]. Available: http://web.b.ebscohost.com/ehost/detail?sid=219f1169-7335-4067-bd03-59f7ac841777%40sessionmgr112&vid=4&hid=113&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#db=pdh&AN=2011-01601-001
[64]
S. Mele, V. Cazzato, and C. Urgesi, ‘The importance of perceptual experience in the esthetic appreciation of the body’, PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 12, Dec. 2013, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081378.
[65]
C. Lambrou, D. Veale, and G. Wilson, ‘The role of aesthetic sensitivity in body dysmorphic disorder.’, Journal of abnormal psychology, vol. 120, no. 2, pp. 443–453, 2011 [Online]. Available: http://web.b.ebscohost.com/ehost/detail?sid=a5eee4ae-b9e0-48d0-9c11-f89eb504af6b%40sessionmgr111&vid=5&hid=122&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#db=pdh&AN=2011-01601-001
[66]
C. Winkler and G. Rhodes, ‘Perceptual adaptation affects attractiveness of female bodies’, British journal of psychology, vol. 96, no. 2, pp. 141–154, May 2005, doi: 10.1348/000712605X36343.
[67]
H. Aviezer, Y. Trope, and A. Todorov, ‘Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions’, Science, vol. 338, no. 6111, pp. 1225–1229, Nov. 2012, doi: 10.1126/science.1224313. [Online]. Available: http://hdi.msu.edu/docs%255CAviezer%2520et%2520al._2012.pdf
[68]
B. de Gelder, ‘Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language’, Nature reviews neuroscience, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 242–249, Mar. 2006, doi: 10.1038/nrn1872.
[69]
C. Urgesi, B. Calvo-Merino, P. Haggard, and S. M. Aglioti, ‘Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals two cortical pathways for visual body processing’, Journal of neuroscience, vol. 27, no. 30, pp. 8023–8030, Jul. 2007, doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0789-07.2007. [Online]. Available: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/27/30/8023
[70]
E. Daprati, M. Iosa, and P. Haggard, ‘A dance to the music of time: aesthetically-relevant changes in body posture in performing art’, PLoS ONE, vol. 4, no. 3, Mar. 2009, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005023.
[71]
I. M. Scott et al., ‘Human preferences for sexually dimorphic faces may be evolutionarily novel’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Sep. 2014, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1409643111.
[72]
B. Calvo-Merino, C. Jola, D. E. Glaser, and P. Haggard, ‘Towards a sensorimotor aesthetics of performing art’, Consciousness and cognition, vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 911–922, Sep. 2008, doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.11.003.
[73]
A. H. Dalziell, R. A. Peters, A. Cockburn, A. D. Dorland, A. C. Maisey, and R. D. Magrath, ‘Dance choreography is coordinated with song repertoire in a complex avian display’, Current biology, vol. 23, no. 12, pp. 1132–1135, Jun. 2013, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.018.
[74]
G. Orgs, N. Hagura, and P. Haggard, ‘Learning to like it: aesthetic perception of bodies, movements and choreographic structure’, Consciousness and cognition, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 603–612, Jun. 2013, doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.03.010.
[75]
G. Orgs, J.-H. Dombrowski, M. Heil, and P. Jansen-Osmann, ‘Expertise in dance modulates alphabeta event-related desynchronization during action observation’, European journal of neuroscience, vol. 27, no. 12, pp. 3380–3384, Jun. 2008, doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06271.x.
[76]
K. McCarty, J. Hönekopp, N. Neave, N. Caplan, and B. Fink, ‘Male body movements as possible cues to physical strength: a biomechanical analysis’, American journal of human biology, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 307–312, May 2013, doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22360.
[77]
E. Cross, ‘The impact of aesthetic evaluation and physical ability on dance perception’, Frontiers in human neuroscience, vol. 5, 2011, doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00102. [Online]. Available: http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00102/full
[78]
Sian L. Beilock and Lauren E. Holt, ‘Embodied preference judgments: can likeability be driven by the motor system?’, Psychological science, vol. 18, no. 1, pp. 51–57, 2007, doi: 10.2307/40064577. [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40064577
[79]
S. Topolinski, ‘Moving the eye of the beholder: motor components in vision determine aesthetic preference’, Psychological science, vol. 21, no. 9, pp. 1220–1224, Sep. 2010, doi: 10.1177/0956797610378308. [Online]. Available: http://pss.sagepub.com/content/21/9/1220
[80]
A. P. Saygin, ‘Point-light biological motion perception activates human premotor cortex’, Journal of neuroscience, vol. 24, no. 27, pp. 6181–6188, Jul. 2004, doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0504-04.2004. [Online]. Available: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/24/27/6181
[81]
B. Calvo-Merino, J. Grèzes, D. E. Glaser, R. E. Passingham, and P. Haggard, ‘Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation’, Current biology, vol. 16, no. 19, pp. 1905–1910, Oct. 2006, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.065.
[82]
G. Rizzolatti and C. Sinigaglia, ‘The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations’, Nature reviews neuroscience, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 264–274, Apr. 2010, doi: 10.1038/nrn2805.
[83]
T. Opacic, C. Stevens, and B. Tillmann, ‘Unspoken knowledge: Implicit learning of structured human dance movement.’, Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, vol. 35, no. 6, pp. 1570–1577, 2009 [Online]. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=pdh&AN=2009-19590-017&site=ehost-live&scope=site&custid=s1123049
[84]
P. Reddish, R. Fischer, and J. Bulbulia, ‘Let’s dance together: synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation’, PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 8, Aug. 2013, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071182.
[85]
M. U. Shankar, C. A. Levitan, and C. Spence, ‘Grape expectations: The role of cognitive influences in color–flavor interactions’, Consciousness and Cognition, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 380–390, Mar. 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.008.
[86]
M. Auvray and C. Spence, ‘The multisensory perception of flavor’, Consciousness and Cognition, vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 1016–1031, Sep. 2008, doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.06.005.
[87]
L. S. Loeken et al., ‘Coding of pleasant touch’, Nature neuroscience, vol. 12, no. 5, pp. 547–548, 2009 [Online]. Available: http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v12/n5/pdf/nn.2312.pdf
[88]
R. Ackerley, K. Saar, F. McGlone, and H. Backlund Wasling, ‘Quantifying the sensory and emotional perception of touch: differences between glabrous and hairy skin’, Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, vol. 8, 2014 [Online]. Available: http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00034/full
[89]
J. Djordjevic, R. J. Zatorre, and M. Jones-Gotman, ‘Odor-induced changes in taste perception’, Experimental brain research, vol. 159, no. 3, pp. 405–408, Dec. 2004, doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2103-y.
[90]
K. Rudenga, B. Green, D. Nachtigal, and D. M. Small, ‘Evidence for an integrated oral sensory module in the human anterior ventral insula’, Chemical senses, vol. 35, no. 8, pp. 693–703, Oct. 2010, doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq068. [Online]. Available: https://login.ezproxy.brunel.ac.uk/login?url=http://chemse.oxfordjournals.org/content/35/8/693.full
[91]
L. Birch, ‘Development of food preferences’, Annual review of nutrition, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 41–62, 1999 [Online]. Available: http://www.annualreviews.org/doi/abs/10.1146/annurev.nutr.19.1.41
[92]
M. Evans, A. Jamal, and Foxall, G. R., Consumer behaviour, 2nd ed. Chichester: Wiley, 2009.
[93]
C. P. Haugtvedt, P. Herr, and Kardes, Frank R., Handbook of consumer psychology, vol. Marketing and consumer psychology series. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2008 [Online]. Available: http://lib.myilibrary.com/browse/open.asp?id=137224&entityid=https://idp.brunel.ac.uk/entity
[94]
C.-C. Carbon, ‘The cycle of preference: Long-term dynamics of aesthetic appreciation’, Acta Psychologica, vol. 134, no. 2, pp. 233–244, Jun. 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.004.
[95]
J. C. Karremans, W. Stroebe, and J. Claus, ‘Beyond Vicary’s fantasies: The impact of subliminal priming and brand choice’, Journal of experimental social psychology, vol. 42, no. 6, pp. 792–798, Nov. 2006, doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.002.
[96]
R. M. Piech et al., ‘Neural correlates of affective influence on choice’, Brain and cognition, vol. 72, no. 2, pp. 282–288, Mar. 2010, doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.09.012.
[97]
E. E. Hannon, G. Soley, and S. Ullal, ‘Familiarity overrides complexity in rhythm perception: A cross-cultural comparison of American and Turkish listeners.’, Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, vol. 38, no. 3, pp. 543–548, 2012 [Online]. Available: http://web.b.ebscohost.com/ehost/detail?vid=3&sid=a9d58374-6742-462f-8ce4-416425400521%40sessionmgr113&hid=120&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#db=pdh&AN=2012-04374-001
[98]
C. Chrea, ‘Semantic, typicality and odor representation: a cross-cultural study’, Chemical senses, vol. 30, no. 1, pp. 37–49, Jan. 2005, doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh255. [Online]. Available: https://login.ezproxy.brunel.ac.uk/login?url=http://chemse.oxfordjournals.org/content/30/1/37.full
[99]
J.-P. Royet, J. Plailly, A.-L. Saive, A. Veyrac, and C. Delon-Martin, ‘The impact of expertise in olfaction’, Frontiers in psychology, vol. 4, 2013, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00928.
[100]
L. P. Kirsch, K. A. Drommelschmidt, and E. S. Cross, ‘The impact of sensorimotor experience on affective evaluation of dance’, Frontiers in human neuroscience, vol. 7, 2013, doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00521.
[101]
C. Delon-Martin, J. Plailly, P. Fonlupt, A. Veyrac, and J.-P. Royet, ‘Perfumers’ expertise induces structural reorganization in olfactory brain regions’, NeuroImage, vol. 68, pp. 55–62, Mar. 2013, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.044.
[102]
A. Castriota-Scanderberg, ‘The appreciation of wine by sommeliers: a functional magnetic resonance study of sensory integration.’, NeuroImage, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 570–578, 2005 [Online]. Available: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811904007062
[103]
B. Calvo-Merino, ‘Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers’, Cerebral cortex, vol. 15, no. 8, pp. 1243–1249, Nov. 2004 [Online]. Available: https://login.ezproxy.brunel.ac.uk/login?url=http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/content/15/8/1243.full
[104]
M. T.F., A. E., and J. L., ‘The musician’s brain as a model of neuroplasticity : Article : Nature Reviews Neuroscience’. [Online]. Available: http://www.nature.com/nrn/journal/v3/n6/full/nrn843.html